Newswise — If you’ve ever seen a seagull snatch some fries or felt their beady eyes on your sandwich in the park, you'd be right to suspect they know exactly when to strike to increase their chances of getting a human snack. 

A new study by the University of Bristol is the most in-depth look to date at the foraging behaviours of urban gulls and how they’ve adapted to patterns of human activity in a city. 

In comparison to natural environments, urban environments are novel for animals on an evolutionary timescale and present a wide array of potential food sources. In urban environments food availability often fluctuates according to patterns of human activity, which can follow a daily or weekly cycle. However, until now, little has been known about how urban animals adapt to these time differences in human food availability. 

A team of scientists from Bristol’s Faculties of Engineering and Life Sciences used different data to record the behaviour of urban gulls at three different settings in the city: a public park, a school and a waste centre. The study used data from mini GPS tracker backpacks fitted to 12 Lesser Black‐backed Gulls, as well as observations of gull numbers at the different sites. 

The team found the birds’ foraging patterns closely matched the timing of school breaks and the opening and closing times of the waste centre, but that their activity in the park appeared to correspond with the availability of natural food sources. 

These findings suggest gulls may have the behavioural flexibility to adapt their foraging behaviour to human time schedules when beneficial, and that this trait helps them to thrive in cities. 

Dr Anouk Spelt, lead author of the paper published in Ibis, the International Journal of Avian Science, said: 

“Our first day at the school, the students were excited to tell us about the gulls visiting their school at lunch time. Indeed, our data showed that gulls were not only present in high numbers during lunch time to feed on leftovers, but also just before the start of the school and during the first break when students had their snack. Similarly, at the waste centre the gulls were present in higher numbers on weekdays when the centre was open and trucks were unloading food waste.

“Although everybody has experienced or seen gulls stealing food from people in parks, our gulls mainly went to park first thing in the morning and this may be because earthworms and insects are present in higher numbers during these early hours.” 

Dr Shane Windsor, co-author, said: 

“With this study in Bristol we have shown that gulls in cities are able to adapt their foraging schedule to make best use of food resources depending on their availability. Some gulls even used all three feeding grounds in the same day, suggesting they might track the availability to optimise their energy intake. These results highlight the behavioural flexibility of gulls and their ability to adapt to the artificial environments and time schedules of urban living.” 

Further information High res images & video can be downloaded here: 

https://fluff.bris.ac.uk/fluff/u1/se17762/qaMo1mph7i4PkccY6nrsGQxpO/

https://fluff.bris.ac.uk/fluff/u1/se17762/H9h__wr5WiddUaW34_CsFAxp0/

 (Credit: University of Bristol) Academic contacts: Lead author, Anouk Spelt: [email protected] Co-author, Dr Shane Windsor: [email protected] Paper: https://doi.org/10.1111/ibi.12892 (onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ibi.12892)  Anouk Spelt1, Oliver Soutar2, Cara Williamson1, Jane Memmott2, Judy Shamoun-Baranes3, Peter Rock1, Shane Windsor1 1Department of Aerospace Engineering, University of Bristol, BS8 1TR, United Kingdom 2School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, BS8 1TQ, United Kingdom 3Institute for Biodiversity and Ecosystem Dynamics, University of Amsterdam, 1098XH, The Netherlands Dr Spelt has also written a blog about the paper: https://www.bou.org.uk/blog-spelt-urban-gull-foraging-schedule/  Funding This work was supported by the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (grant number 679355), the University Research Committee (URC)/Institute for Advanced Studies (IAS) Interdisciplinary Research Internship Scheme (IRIS) 2018, and the EPSRC Centre for Doctoral Training in Future Autonomous and Robotic Systems (FARSCOPE) at the Bristol Robotics Laboratory. This work was carried out on the Dutch national e-infrastructure with the support of SURF Cooperative.

Journal Link: IBIS